perm filename CHAP1[4,KMC]9 blob
sn#053957 filedate 1973-07-12 generic text, type T, neo UTF8
00100 .SEC THE PARANOID MODE
00200 .SS The Concept of Paranoia
00300 Like ourselves, the ancient Greeks called one another
00400 paranoid. The term `paranoia' (Gr.:para=beside; nous = mind) was a
00500 lay rather than a medical term which referred to states of craziness
00600 and mental deterioration. For two thousand years the term did not
00700 appear in the classification schemes of mental disorders. (Menninger
00800 et al.) Historians do not seem curious about what persons with
00900 persecutory delusions were called all that time. (It is doubtful that
01000 there weren't any.) In the 18th century the term reappears in German
01100 classifications to refer to delusional states which were considered
01200 disorders of intellect rather than emotion. (Lewis ).
01300 Little agreement about the term's usuage was reached until
01400 the present time when it has achieved adjectival status as in
01500 "paranoid personality" and "paranoid state". Currently the term is
01600 used to refer to the presence of persecutory delusions. Somatic
01700 erotic, grandeur and jealousy delusions are simply identified as such
01800 without calling them paranoid.
01900
02000 .SS Characteristics of Clinical Paranoias
02100 In dividing the world of experience into conceptual classes ,
02200 we also sort and group objects and events together according to
02300 properties they have in common. The members of a class resemble one
02400 another in sharing certain properties. The resemblance between
02500 members of a class is not exact or total. Members of a conceptual
02600 class are considered more or less alike and there exist degrees of
02700 resemblance. Humans are neither subjective nor objective, they are
02800 projective. In forming classifications we project our interests and
02900 intentions onto the world. Thus the world of experience consists of
03000 relations, not of objects isolated from human intentions.
03100 Observations and classifications made by clinicians regarding
03200 paranoia have been thoroughly described in the psychiatric
03300 literature. Extensive accounts can be found in Swanson, Bohnert and
03400 Smith (1970) and in Cameron (1967). I shall attempt to give a
03500 condensed description of paranoid phenomena as they appear in, or are
03600 described by, patients in a psychiatric interview.
03700
03800 The phenomena the model attempts to explain can be described
03900 under concepts of suspiciousness, self-reference, hypersensitivity,
04000 fearfulness , hostility and rigidity - these properties representing
04100 empirical indicators of paranoia.
04200
04300 .F Suspiciousness
04400 The main characteristic of the clinical paranoid mode
04500 consists of suspiciousness of others which derives from the patient's
04600 malevolence beliefs. The patient believes others, known and
04700 unknown, have evil intentions towards him. He is continously on the
04800 look-out for signs of malevolence which he often reads from his own
04900 probings. He is hypervigilant; people must be watched, their schemes
05000 unmasked and foiled. He is convinced others try to bring about
05100 undesirable states in himself such as humiliation, harassment,
05200 subjugation, injury and even death. In an interview he may report
05300 such beliefs directly or ,if he is guarded , they will only be hinted
05400 at. He does not confide easily. Disclosure may depend upon how the
05500 interviewer responds in the dialogue to reports of fluctuating
05600 suspicions or of absolute convictions of malevolence.
05700 He is greatly concerned with "evidence". No room is allowed
05800 for mistakes, ambiguities or chance happenings. "Paranoids have a
05900 greater passion for the truth than other madmen " -(Bellow in
06000 Sammler's Planet). Using trivial evidential details, he leaps from
06100 the undeniable to the unbelievable.
06200 The patient may vary in his own estimate of the strength of
06300 his malevolence beliefs. If they consist of weakly-held suspicions,
06400 he may have moments of reasoning with himself in which he tries to
06500 reject them as ill-founded. But when the beliefs represent absolute
06600 convictions, he does not struggle to dismiss them. They become
06700 pre-conditions for countering actions against tormentors who wish and
06800 try to do him evil. He seeks affirmation of his beliefs, sympathy ,
06900 and allies in positions of power such as clinicians or lawyers who
07000 can help him take action. "It is certain that my conviction increases
07100 the moment another soul will believe in it." -(Conrad in Lord Jim).
07200 The conceptual content of the malevolence beliefs may involve
07300 a specific other person or a conspiracy of others such as the Mafia,
07400 the FBI, Communists, Hell's Angels. The patient sees himself as a
07500 victim ,one who suffers at the hands of others rather than as an
07600 agent who brings the suffering on himself. Other agents subject him
07700 to and make him the object of their evil intentions. He dwells on and
07800 rehearses in his mind these outrages. He schemes to defeat or escape
07900 his adversaries. The misdeeds of others are denounced, desparaged,
08000 condemned and belittled. He feels interfered with and discriminated
08100 against. At times the conceptual content of the beliefs are not
08200 directly expressed in an interview. The patient may be so
08300 mistrustful of how their disclosure might be used against him that he
08400 cautiously feels his way through an interview offering only hints
08500 which the clinician then uses to infer the presence of delusions.
08600
08700 Using his own judgements about the truth-status of beliefs, a
08800 clinician attempts to determine whether the malevolence beliefs
08900 expressed are true or false. This diagnosis is usually not difficult
09000 in clinical settings. Occasionally malevolence beliefs turn out to be
09100 true. Others are true but have derived from the paranoid's tendency
09200 to accuse and provoke others to the point where they in fact display
09300 hostility towards him.
09400
09500
09600 .F Self-Reference and Hypersensitivity
09700
09800
09900 The patient believes many more events in the world pertain to
10000 himself in a negative way than seems justified to other observers.
10100 For example, he is convinced that newspaper headlines are directed
10200 personally at him or that the statements of radio announcers contain
10300 special messages for him. In his hypervigilance he hypersensitively
10400 reads himself into situations which are not actually intended to
10500 pertain to him and his particular concerns. The references to
10600 the self are usually interpreted as malevolent. He may
10700 believe he is being observed and mysteriously influenced by others
10800 with evil intentions. He is excessively concerned about eyes (which
10900 can both watch and punish), cameras, telescopes ,etc. which may be
11000 directed his way.
11100
11200 References to the self are misconstrued as slurs, slights or
11300 unfair judgements. He may feel he is being watched, stared at and
11400 even mysteriously influenced. He is hypersensitive to criticism. In
11500 crowds he believes he is intentionally bumped and on the highway he
11600 feels repeatedly tail-gaited. Badgered and bombarded without relief
11700 by this stream of wrongs , he becomes hyperirritable, querulous and
11800 guarrelsome.
11900 He is touchy about certain topics, flaring up when references
12000 to particular conceptual domains appear in the conversation. For
12100 example, any remarks about his age, religion, or family, or sexlife
12200 may set him off. Even when these domains are touched upon without
12300 reference to him, e.g. religion in general, he takes it personally.
12400 When a delusional complex is present, linguistic terms far removed
12500 from, but still connectable to, the complex, stir him up. Thus, to a
12600 man holding beliefs that the Mafia intend to harm him, a remark about
12700 Italy might lead him to react in a suspicious or fearful manner.
12800
12900 .F Affect-States
13000
13100 The major affects expressed, both verbally and nonverbally,
13200 are those of fear and anger. The patient may be fearful of physical
13300 attack and injury even to the point of death. He fears that others
13400 wish to subjugate and control him. His fear is justified in his mind
13500 by the many physical threats he detects in the conduct of others
13600 towards him. He is hostile to what are interpreted as insinuations
13700 or demeaning allusions. His chronic irritability becomes punctuated
13800 with outbursts of raging tirades and diatribes. When he feels he is
13900 being overwhelmed he may erupt and in desperation physically attack
14000 others.
14100 The experienced and expressed affects of fear and anger blend
14200 with one another in varying proportions to yield an unpleasant
14300 negative affect state made continuous by fantasied rehearsals and
14400 retellings of past wrongs. Depending on his interpretation of
14500 malevolent input, the patient may move away from others in being
14600 guarded, secretive and evasive and then may suddenly jump at others
14700 with sarcastic accusations and arguments. His affect-states
14800 become chained into loops with those others in his life space who
14900 take censoring action towards him because of his uncommunicativeness
15000 or outbursts.
15100
15200 As will be described, the affects of shame and humiliation
15300 are postulated to play a crucial role in the operations of the
15400 paranoid mode. One of the assumptions of the theory to be
15500 presented involves a principle of escaping humiliation to prevent a
15600 further reduction in self-esteem through re-experienced humiliation.
15700
15800 .F Rigidity
15900
16000 Among the chief properties of clinically observed paranoia
16100 are those phenomena which may be characterized as indicators of
16200 rigidity. The patient's beliefs in his sensitive areas remain fixed,
16300 difficult to influence by evidence or persuasion. The patient
16400 himself makes few verification attempts which might disconfirm his
16500 convictions. To change a belief is to admit being wrong. To
16600 forgive others also opens a crack in the wall of righteousness. He
16700 does not apologize nor accept apology. He stubbornly follows rules
16800 to the letter and his literal interpretations of regulations can
16900 drive others wild. It is this stance or posture of rigidity and
17000 inflexibility which makes the treatment of paranoia by
17100 symbolic-semantic methods so difficult.
17200
17300 .SS Theories of Paranoia
17400
17500 While paranoid processes represent a disorder from one
17600 standpoint, the observable regularities of the disorder imply an
17700 underlying nonrandom "pathological" order at another level.
17800 Attempts to explain, to make intelligible, the order behind the
17900 disorder, have been offered since antiquity. None of these
18000 verbally-stated formulations have won the consensus characteristic of
18100 scientific theories since they were neither systematic nor testable.
18200 Science represents a search for consensible knowledge,
18300 judgements about which agreement can be obtained under particular
18400 requirements. The major requirements for a theory to reach consensus
18500 involve systemicity and testability.
18600 For a theory to be systematic, its hypotheses must cohere,
18700 not be isolated, connect with one another and collaborate in a
18800 consistent way. Each hypothesis stands as an initial assumption or as
18900 a consequence of one or more initial assumptions. The consequence
19000 relation can be one of logical or empirical entailment but the
19100 system, to be consistent, cannot contain contradictions.
19200 For a theory to be testable, it must be sensitive to
19300 empirical data which can strenghten or weaken its acceptability. Each
19400 hypothesis in the theory need not be directly or independently
19500 testable. But the theory as a conjunction of hypotheses must be
19600 brought into contact with data of observation, if not directly, then
19700 indirectly, through a translation in which a consequence of the
19800 theory can be compared with observational evidence.
19900 Previous theories of paranoia are to be criticized for not
20000 satisfying these requirements of systemicity and testability. The
20100 model to be presented fulfills these requirements. When theories are
20200 presented in literary form it is difficult to know what such
20300 formulations imply or whether the implications are consistent. They
20400 do not tell us what we are supposed to do in order to replicate the
20500 experience of their authors. If a formulation is untestable, the
20600 issues it raises are undecidable and unsettleable and consensus
20700 cannot be reached.
20800
20900 Theories stem from two sources, from hypotheses suggested by
21000 new descriptions (revisualizations) of the phenomena themselves and
21100 from modifications of a legacy of previous theories serving as the
21200 bequeathed myths of the field. Each generation develops new
21300 explanatory theories by discovering new phenomena or by modifying
21400 predecessor theories. The old theories are unsatisfactory or only
21500 partially satisfactory because they are found to contain anomalies or
21600 contradictions which must be removed. Sometimes previous theories are
21700 viewed as lacking evidential support by current standards. Theories
21800 are mainly superseded rather than disproved. The new versions try
21900 to remove the contradictions and increase comprehensiveness by
22000 explaining more phenomena.
22100 Theories have many functions. They can be summarized as
22200 follows ( Bunge) .V
22300 (1)To systematize knowledge.
22400 (2)To explain facts by showing how they are the entailed
22500 consequences of the systematizing hypotheses.
22600 (3)To increase knowledge by deriving new facts.
22700 (4)To enhance the testability of hypotheses by connecting
22800 them to observations.
22900 (5)To guide research by:
23000 (a) posing fruitful problems
23100 (b) suggesting new data to gather
23200 (c) opening new lines of investigation
23300 (6)To map a portion of reality. .END
23400 It is a tall order for a theory to fulfill all of these
23500 functions. In undeveloped fields we should be happy with even one of
23600 them. Models can be assigned these functions when they are
23700 theoretical in type. Our model was intended primarily to serve
23800 functions (2) and (4), testable explanation.
23900 Theories offered as scientific explanations should be (a)
24000 systematic (i.e. coherent and consistent) and (b) empirically
24100 testable. Prior psychological formulations about the paranoid mode do
24200 not meet these criteria and thus are deservedly bygone notions. For
24300 example, to account for paranoid thought process by citing an
24400 imbalance of intellect and affect or defective role-taking is to be
24500 so global and untestable as to not merit theory status. To gain
24600 consensus with an explanation of the right type we need a structure
24700 of symbol-manipulating processes, strategies, functions or procedures
24800 which is capable of producing the observable regularities of the
24900 paranoid mode.
25000 In psychiatry it is still useful to view some things which
25100 happen to a man in mechanical terms. But a man is not only a passive
25200 recipient, subject to Newtons's laws, he is also an active agent, a
25300 language user who thereby can monitor himself, control himself,
25400 direct himself, comment on and criticize these performances. Modern
25500 psychiatric theory should try to offer an account of man as agent as
25600 well as recipient and come to grips with those enigmatic cases in
25700 which what happens to a man can be a consequence of what he has done.
25800 Let us consider some psychological explanations for the
25900 paranoid mode beginning in the late 19th century. (Historians can
26000 certainly find notions of intentions, affects and beliefs as far back
26100 as Aristotle, who seldom quoted his sources. "Its all been said
26200 before but you have to say it again because nobody listens"(Gide)).
26300 In 1896 Freud [ ] proposed a formulation which postulated a defensive
26400 strategy or procedure to explain persecutory paranoia . He assumed
26500 the believed persecution of the self by others to represent projected
26600 self-reproaches for childhood masturbation. Today hardly anyone
26700 finds this explanation plausible. While it has withered, however,
26800 the concepts of defense and projection have weathered the years.
26900 Around the turn of the century, Fliess (according to Jones[
27000 ]) proposed in letters to Freud that paranoid thought stemmed from an
27100 unconscious homosexual conflict. Freud embraced this idea
27200 whole-heartedly and in 1910 [ ] developed it in terms of
27300 transformations being applied to the basic proposition `I (a man)
27400 love him.' He postulated this proposition to be so intolerable as not
27500 to be admitted to consciousness and therefore subjected to
27600 unconscious transformations, first into `I do not love him, I hate
27700 him' which in turn was transformed into the conscious belief `He
27800 hates me' with the accompanying conclusion `Therefore I am justified
27900 in hating him'. Great difficulty has been encountered in testing the
28000 theory since there is no agreed-on method for detecting the presence
28100 of unconscious homosexual conflict. The explanation is also
28200 inconsistent with another of Freud's ideas that everyone harbors
28300 unconscious homosexual conflicts. But not everyone becomes paranoid.
28400 To reconcile the inconsistency one would have to postulate some
28500 additional, possibly quantitative factors, to explain the intensity
28600 and extent of the paranoid mode in certain people. Another
28700 difficulty is the fact that overtly homosexual people can be
28800 paranoid, requiring in such cases a postulate of some other type of
28900 underlying conflict.
29000 Because of its inconsistency and untestability the
29100 homosexual-conflict explanation has not achieved consensus. But as
29200 will be discussed, it may contain a grain of truth as a limiting case
29300 for a more general hypotheses regarding humiliation in the paranoid
29400 mode. Freud's later attempts at the explanation of paranoia (In Ego
29500 and Id) assumed simply that love was transformed into hate. This
29600 notion is too vague and incomplete an articulation to qualify for a
29700 contemporary explanation which reqires a more complex and precisely
29800 defined organization of functions to account for such a
29900 transformation.
30000 Likewise Cameron's explanation (1967) of "projected hostilty"
30100 represents a single, isolated hypothesis lacking systemicity and
30200 insufficiently complex to match the complexity of paranoid phenomena.
30300 Tomkins [ ] in 1963 offered an arresting
30400 information-processing theory of the paranoid posture articulated in
30500 terms of defensive strategies, transformations ,and
30600 maximizing-minimizing principles. He viewed the paranoid "posture" or
30700 mode as an attempt to cope with humiliation. He proposed that a
30800 person whose information processing is monopolized by the paranoid
30900 mode is in a permanent state of vigilance, trying to maximize the
31000 detection of insult and to minimize humiliation.
31100 Regarding the paranoid, Tomkins stated:
31200 .V
31300 "The major source of distortion in his interpretation is
31400 in his insistence on processing all information as though
31500 it were relevant only to the possibility of humiliation."
31600 .END
31700 In 1970 Swanson et al.[ ] in a book on paranoia portrayed how
31800 a `homeostatic' individual attempts to deal with `bewildering
31900 perceptions'. They postulated that a person in homeostatic
32000 equilibrium perceives a pronounced inner or outer change which is
32100 inexplicable or unacceptable. The resultant disequilibrium is so
32200 bewildering that in order to restore equilibrium, the person
32300 constructs a paranoid explanation which attributes the cause of the
32400 change, not to the self, but to an external source. With the cause
32500 of the change identified , bewilderment is abolished and uncertainty
32600 reduced.
32700 Elements of this formulation represent symbol-processing
32800 strategies particularly in cases of paranoid thinkng associated with
32900 the `pronounced changes' of organic brain damage or amphetamine
33000 psychosis. These are conditions which happen to a man. In paranoid
33100 states, reactions or personalities where no pronounced physical
33200 change can be identified ,the formulation is inadequate and must be
33300 filled out with more complex and specific processes.
33400 In sum, the rival theories of paranoia here reviewed have not
33500 gained widespread acceptance because of various weaknesses and
33600 limitations. No reigning and unified theory of paranoia is widely
33700 accepted today. In such a pre-consensus state, the field is wide open
33800 for contenders. Previous theories have contributed useful hypotheses.
33900 Some of them will be incorporated in a new attempt to explain
34000 paranoid phenomen in a different way, using the strategy of
34100 simulation. I shall attempt to to explain sequences of paranoid
34200 symbolic behavior (conversational interactions) by describing in some
34300 detail a simulation of paranoid interview behavior , having in mind
34400 an audience of mental health professionals and colleagues in fields
34500 of psychiatry, psychology, artificial intelligence, linguistics and
34600 philosophy. The simulation model proposed (Colby, Weber and
34700 Hilf,1971) stands as a candidate explanation intended to be more
34800 explicit, systematic, consistent and testable than the theories
34900 described above. The model combines hypotheses of previous
35000 formulations e.g.Tomkins' hypotheses regarding humiliation, with
35100 hypotheses and assumptions of my own, into a coherent, unified
35200 explanatory system. Let us next consider the nature of explanatory
35300 systems.